Social and Emotional Development In Child-Care Settings

A significant proportion of children spend at least some time in non-parental care during their first five years of life. This chapter focuses on aspects of non-parental care that shape the development of social and emotional competencies in young children (Figure 4.1).

Similar to parent-child interactions, interactions that children have with non-parental caregivers can play an important role in promoting child development. This may be driven by the education level and training of the provider, as well as the overall quality of the non-parental care. The types of child care, including center-based, family child-care homes, relative care, or care in the home by a nonrelative — differ in their level of formality, the way they are set up, and the qualifications of the caregivers. This suggests that there may be unique needs for each setting to ensure that all children in Shelby County are receiving care in a way that maximizes their social and emotional development.

We focus much of the discussion in this chapter on formal (typically licensed) child-care providers, including center-based and family child care home settings. However, these issues can also apply to informal settings.

Circles of Influence Figure 4.1

Formal child-care providers offer two main types of support important for children’s social and emotional development 40, 41.

  • Instructional support provides learning experiences or encourages skill development through interactions between a child and a child-care provider.
  • Emotional support, defined by the warmth and sensitivity provided to the child, encourages the development of social and emotional competencies through responsive and supportive interactions 40, 42.

Data facts:

  • Nationally, only 13 percent of preschool teachers get high ratings on instructional support 43.
  • Nationally, fewer than half of preschool teachers are rated as having a high level of emotional support 43.

Why are provider-child interactions important?

The interactions between children and their child-care providers contribute to the development of children’s emerging social and emotional skills.

Positive provider-child relationships in early childhood often include a high degree of warmth and closeness, a minimal amount of conflict, and can occur when the child is able to share the provider with other children in the group setting 42-45.

Children who are in high-quality formal child care have:

  • Increased social skills at the end of the preschool year 42
  • Improved academic school readiness 43
  • Less problem behavior at school entry and at the beginning of high school 42, 46
  • Better academic outcomes, which persist from school entry to the end of high school 46.

Where do children spend their time?

Many children spend a significant amount of time in the care of someone other than their parents or guardians. Nationally, about 60 percent of children ages five and under who are not yet enrolled in school are in some form of non-parental child care on a weekly basis 48, 49 (Figure 4.2).

Where do Children Spend Their Time? Figure 4.2
Percentage of children birth through five, nationally. Note: Categories are not mutually exclusive
Source: Mamedova, S., and J. Redford, Early childhood program participation, from the National Household Education Surveys Program of 2012 (NCES 2013-029). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, Washington, D.C., 2013
Who Receives Regular Nonparental Care? Figure 4.3
Percentage of children, by age
Source: Table 1 from Mamedova and Redford (2013) 49
Who Provides the Care? Figure 4.4
Where children spend their time when away from parents
Source: Table 1 from Mamedova and Redford (2013) 49

The use of non-parental care and the type of non-parental care most typically used by parents varies by the age of the child (Figure 4.3). Generally, use of care—and more specifically, use of center-based care—is greater for older children, ages 3 and 4 (Figure 4.4). By then, most children are in center-based settings on a regular basis 49.

Data fact:

  • Preschool-age children spend about 33 hours per week in child care 48.

What do we know about child-care quality in Shelby County?

Child-care quality is hard to measure in Shelby County because there is not a standard way to assess it. Currently, two programs are used to assess licensed child care quality above minimum licensing requirements: the Tennessee-initiated Star-Quality Child Care Program and National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) accreditation. Participation in either rating system is voluntary. Although measures of provider-child interactions are not assessed directly as part of the Star-Quality program, higher standards present in three-star providers or NAEYC-accredited centers may be more likely to foster positive provider-child interactions or promote positive social and emotional development. However, more work is needed to validate these rating scales with respect to social and emotional outcomes of children.

Star Rating Levels Table 4.1
Among licensed providers participating in the Star-Quality program serving ages zero to five, by type of care

Data facts:

  • About 67 percent of eligible Shelby County licensed child-care providers participate in the Star-Quality rating program. Of these, 87 percent of center-based providers have a three-star rating, and 78 percent of family child-care providers have a three-star rating. Table 4.1 shows the distribution of the star ratings among providers who participated.
  • In Shelby County, only about 7 percent of all licensed centers are accredited by NAEYC.

Sources: Tennessee Child Care Management System for September 2014, provided by Child Care ReSource and Referral (LeBonheur Community Health and Well Being Division); NAEYC Accredited Program Search, http://www.naeyc.org/academy/accreditation/search

Where are three-star providers located?

It is helpful to understand the geographic distribution of higher-quality licensed child care across the county to identify areas of need and potential models of success. While this is difficult to assess due to the voluntary nature of the quality rating systems, we can get a glimpse into this by looking at child-care providers who participated in the Star-Quality rating system. The map in Figure 4.5 shows the proportion of child-care slots in each zip code that are in three-star providers. This map does not include child-care providers that did not participate in the Star-Quality rating system, including centers that are NAEYC-accredited but do not have a Star-Quality rating. As a result, it provides a limited look at child-care quality in Shelby County. Zip codes with the lowest proportion of three-star child-care slots include 38122 (54 percent), 38125 (72 percent), 38134 (72 percent), 38108 (78 percent), and 38128 (79 percent).

Three-star Child-care Providers Figure 4.5
Percentage of child-care slots with a three-star rating in Shelby County among child-care providers that participated in the Star-Quality rating system, by zip code. Note: No providers in 38028 currently participate in the rating system
Source: Tennessee Child Care Management System for September 2014, provided by Child Care ReSource and Referral (LeBonheur Community Health and Well Being Division); Capacity numbers provided through Department of Human Services Child-Care Providers Zip Code List—Shelby County (http://www.tn.gov/accweb/faces/stateMapPage.jsp)

Conclusion

Children spend a substantial part of their time in non-parental care both inside and outside the home. Because of this, it is important to understand the current state of provider-child interactions that promote social and emotional health. Though little data exist to examine this locally, two quality-rating systems provide some insight. However, these systems have limitations, as well. First, they are voluntary, meaning there is no centralized data to examine child-care quality across Shelby County. Second, the Tennessee Star-Quality program does not explicitly capture provider-child interactions, which we know to be important. Third, they are not used to assess quality of provider-child interactions in more informal settings. Finding ways to improve child-care quality—and our measurement of quality—will increase the number of children who have access to high-quality care in Shelby County.

References: 
  1. Gilmore, J.H., Lin, W., Prastawa, M.W., Looney, C.B., Sampath, Y., Vetsa, K., et al. (2007). Regional gray matter growth, sexual dimorphism, and cerebral asymmetry in the neonatal brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(6), 1255–1260.
  2. Nowakowski, R.S. (2006). Stable neuron numbers from cradle to grave. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(33), 12219–12220.
  3. Fox, S.E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C.A., III. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Development, 81(1), 28–40.
  4. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2007). The timing and quality of early experiences combine to shape brain architecture. Working Paper No. 5.
  5. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
  6. Putnam, S.P., Sanson, A.V., & Rothbart, M.K. (2002). Child temperament and parenting. Handbook of Parenting, 1, 255–277.
  7. Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1979). Infant–mother attachment. American Psychologist, 34(10), p. 932.
  8. Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: Assessed in the strange situation and at home. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  9. Sroufe, L.A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attachment & Human Development, 7(4), pp. 349–367.
  10. Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A. (2005). Placing early attachment experiences in developmental context. In K. E. Grossmann, K. Grossmann, & E. Waters (Eds.), The power of longitudinal attachment research: From infancy and childhood to adulthood. New York: Guilford, pp. 48 – 70.
  11. Illinois Early Learning Project website. (2014). Retrieved September 2014, from: http://illinoisearlylearning.org/
  12. Vogel, C.A., Boller, K., Xue, Y., Blair, R., Aikens, N., Burwick, A., et al. (2011). Learning as we go: A first snapshot of Early Head Start programs, staff, families, and children. OPRE Report #2011-7, Washington, D.C.: Office of Planning, Research, and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
  13. Heckman, J.J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312(5782), 1900–1902
  14. Morse, S.B., Zheng, H., Tang, Y., & Roth, J. (2009). Early school-age outcomes of late preterm infants. Pediatrics, 123(4), e622–e629.
  15. Cepeda, I.L., Grunau, R.E., Weinberg, H., Herdman, A.T., Cheung, T., Liotti, M., et al. (2007). Magneto-encephalography study of brain dynamics in young children born extremely preterm. International Congress Series, 1300, 99–102.
  16. Figlio, D.N., Guryan, J., Karbownik, K., & Roth, J. (2013). The effects of poor neonatal health on children’s cognitive development. National Bureau of Economic Research.
  17. Phillips, D.A. & Shonkoff, J.P. (2000). From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. National Academies Press.
  18. Moore, K.A., Redd, Z., Burkhauser, M., Mbwana, K., & Collins, A. (2002) Children in poverty: Trends, consequences and policy options. Child Trends, 2009-11.
  19. Eamon, M.K. (2000). Structural model of the effects of poverty on externalizing and internalizing behaviors of four-tofive-year-old children. Social Work Research, 24(3), 143–154.
  20. Yeung, W.J., Linver, M.R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). How money matters for young children’s development: parental investment and family processes. Child Development, 73(6), 1861–1879.
  21. Sklar, C. (2010). Charting a new course for children in poverty: The reauthorization of the Temporary Assistance for Needy Families Program. Washington D.C.: Zero to Three.
  22. Davis-Kean, P.E. (2005). The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: The indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 294.
  23. Carlson, M.J. & Corcoran, M.E. (2001). Family structure and children’s behavioral and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(3), 779–792.
  24. McLanahan, S.S. (1995). The consequences of nonmarital child-bearing for women, children, and society. Report to Congress on out-of-wedlock childbearing. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
  25. Kaye, K. (2012). Why it matters: Teen childbearing and infant health. Washington, D.C.: The National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy.
  26. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Massachussetts: Harvard University Press.
  27. Sabol, T.J., & Pianta, R.C. (2012). Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and socioemotional development at the end of elementary school. Child Development, 83(1), 282–299.
  28. Raikes, H.A. & Thompson, R.A. (2006). Family emotional climate, attachment security and young children’s emotion knowledge in a high risk sample. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 89–104.
  29. Rice, K.F. & Groves, B.M. (2005). Hope and healing: A caregiver’s guide to helping young children affected by trauma. Washington, D.C.: Zero to Three.
  30. Cummings, E.M., Schermerhorn, A.C., Keller, P.S., & Davies, P.T. (2008). Parental depressive symptoms, children’s representations of family relationships, and child adjustment. Social Development, 17(2), 278–305.
  31. Elgar, F.J., Mills, R.S.L., McGrath, P.J., Waschbusch, D.A., & Brownridge, D.A. (2007). Maternal and paternal depressive symptoms and child maladjustment: The mediating role of parental behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(6), 943–955.
  32. Goodman, S.H., & Gotlib, I.H. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review, 106(3), 458.
  33. Lim, J., Wood, B.L., & Miller, B.D. (2008). Maternal depression and parenting in relation to child internalizing symptoms and asthma disease activity. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(2), 264.
  34. Lovejoy, M.C., Graczyk, P.A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(5) 561–592.
  35. Gaynes, B.N., Gavin, N., Meltzer-Brody, S., Lohr, K.N., Swinson, T., Gartlehner, G., & Miller, W.C. (2005). Perinatal depression: Prevalence, screening accuracy, and screen- ing outcomes. Evidence report/technology assessment (summary), 119, 1–8.
  36. United States Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families. (2011). Early Head Start Research and Evaluation (EHSRE) Study, 1996-2010 Ann Arbor, Mich.: Interuniversity Consortium for Political and Social Research.
  37. Shaw, D.S., Owens, E.B., Giovannelli, J., & Winslow, E.B. (2001). Infant and toddler pathways leading to early externalizing disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(1), 36–43.
  38. Latimer, K., Wilson, P., Kemp, J. Thompson, L., Sim, F., Gillberg, C., et al. (2012). Disruptive behaviour disorders: A systematic review of environmental antenatal and early years risk factors. Child: Care, Health and Development, 38(5), 611–628.
  39. Manuel, J.I., Martinson, M.L., Bledsoe-Mansori, S.E., & Bellamy, J.L. (2012). The influence of stress and social support on depressive symptoms in mothers with young children. Social Science & Medicine, 75(11) 2013–2020.
  40. La Paro, K.M., Pianta, R.C. , & Stuhlman, M. (2004). The classroom assessment scoring system: Findings from the prekindergarten year. The Elementary School Journal, 104(5), 409–426.
  41. Pianta, R., Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Bryant, D., Clifford, R., Early, D., & Barbarin, O. (2005). Features of pre-kindergarten programs, classrooms, and teachers: Do they predict observed classroom quality and child-teacher interactions? Applied Developmental Science, 9(3), 144–159.
  42. Mashburn, A.J., Pianta, R.C., Hamre, B.K., Downer, J.T., Barbarin, O.A., Bryant, D., et al. (2008). Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children’s development of academic, language, and social skills. Child Development, 79(3), 732–749.
  43. Burchinal, M., Vandergrift, N., Pianta, R., & Mashburn, A. (2010). Threshold analysis of association between child care quality and child outcomes for low-income children in pre-kindergarten programs. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(2), 166–176.
  44. Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Bryant, D., Early, D., Clifford, R., et al. (2008). Ready to learn? Children’s pre-academic achievement in pre-kindergarten programs. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(1), 27–50.
  45. Sabol, T.J., & Pianta, R.C. (2012). Recent trends in research on teacher-child relationships. Attachment & Human Development, 14(3), 213–231.
  46. Vandell, D.L., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Vandergrift, N., & Steinberg, L. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. Child Development, 81(3), 737–756.
  47. Hamre, B.K, (2012). A Course on Effective Teacher-Child Interactions Effects on Teacher Beliefs, Knowledge, and Observed Practice. American Education Research Journal, 49(1), 88–123.
  48. Laughlin, L. (2011). Who’s Minding the Kids?: Child Care Arrangements. U.S. Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census.
  49. Mamedova, S., & Redford, J. (2013). Early childhood program participation, from the National Household Education Surveys Program of 2012 (NCES 2013-029). Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.